Why does culture increased adaptability




















He knows that to do so, would be to suggest that he sees the app as more important than his colleagues. Cultural Competence requires the ability to address the fact that we see 'others' in particular ways. Photo by Adrian Dascal on Unsplash. Joe has also asked someone in the office to arrange a lunch to enable a further opportunity for relationships building. Again — he avoids any talk of business during the lunch.

He has arranged a further meeting the following day to discuss the app. Joe knows that if he asks direct questions, as he would back in the USA, he will not get him the answers he needs.

Disagreement or dislike would then be communicated using non-verbal means. He is culturally aware and understands that the way he does things may be different to the way his colleagues do things elsewhere in the company. Not only does he understand this, but he also respects these differences. Taking this one step further, Joe then uses his understanding to adapt his approach to get the best out of his interactions.

Clearly, when he returns to the USA , he will probably revert to the every-day approach which has been shaped by the culture in which he has been raised. So, to summarise, adaptation is an essential part of cultural competence. Being versatile is a requirement for many modern roles, such as those in client-facing situations collaborating with different working cultures. Being able to successfully negotiate these differences ultimately makes for much happier and more fruitful business relationships.

It might be part of your wider diversity and inclusion objectives to create not only a diverse workforce, but an inclusive workforce. Being culturally adaptable improves office cultural versatility; a vital and very profitable talent for any sales team.

Adapting to different clients and environments, where even normalised actions like handshakes could cause offence, is a key skill for any 21st Century organisation. While one of your suppliers might adopt very informal greetings, another might find this completely inappropriate. Having this cultural intelligence and adaptability means you can work effectively and amicably with a diverse base of clients and shareholders alike.

With recent studies suggesting that long-term corporate growth is now tied to integrating adaptive organisation culture, the question remains; how has your organisation prepared for the future?

In a world where business leaders are prioritising intercultural cooperation , adapting to cultural differences in business is more important than ever. Our Cultural Adaptability training is suitable for all staff, especially for those — such as sales teams, managers, leaders and front-line staff — who work with clients, stakeholders and customers with potentially different cultural norms.

Clare Cromarty is a Diversity, Organisational Development and HR consultant for EW Group, having worked across the sectors to build clients' understanding of practical commitments to diversity and inclusion through their approach to organisational development, HR practice and employee engagement.

Clare specialises in reviewing HR practices and supporting clients through challenging HR situations, ensuring values, integrity and inclusivity are at the heart of decisions. Cultural Adaptability is our ability to recognise, understand and work effectively within a range of different contexts where there are different cultural norms. EW Group specialise in the development of inclusive cultures through cross-cultural training.

You can train your teams, individuals or entire departments, at all levels of your organisation. Subscribe for access to leading-edge diversity practice, opinion, insight and exclusive event invites. Press enter to begin your search. How to adapt communication for cultural differences, and why is it so important? No Comments. Cultural adaptability training Maybe you are already aware of cultural clashes within your own organisation that need addressing. Why do we need to be culturally adaptable?

Adapting communication for cultural differences Language is an important aspect to consider when working across new cultural geographies and learning how to adapt communication for cultural differences.

This is often a concern for conservation biologists interested in mitigating the effects of human activity on plant and animal populations 11 , However, in the case of human evolution itself, an understanding of adaptation to drastically changing environments might also help us to elucidate the history of our species.

Why, for example, is the human species so good at moving, and adapting, to such a variety of extremely diverse habitats when other great ape species are not?

Here we suggest that beneficial cultural traits act in two ways to help human populations survive drastic environmental shifts. First, the presence of the cultural trait can itself compensate for a mismatch with the environment and mitigate the consequences of genetic maladaptation.

And second, the presence of even a weakly beneficial cultural trait might allow a population to survive longer with a slowly declining population, increasing the probability of a true evolutionary rescue—a possibility usually discounted in species with long generation times such as ours.

Here we describe models of cultural adaptation that examine the probability of adaptation from standing cultural variation compared to that for adaptation from novel innovation. We examine the implications for human evolution in general and discuss the importance of including gene-culture co-evolution in models of human adaptation in particular.

Finally, we discuss what role cultural adaptation might play in evolutionary rescue and the preservation and persistence of human populations faced with local extinction.

In the following we consider a cultural trait with two alternative variants, a and A , where A is ancestral and a represents a novel innovation. We calculate probability of fixation for the novel innovation in two different adaptation scenarios: adaptation from de novo innovation and adaptation from standing variation.

Adaptation from de novo innovation occurs where the variant a arose in a single individual i. Adaptation from standing variation occurs where the novel variant a was present in the population at some frequency at the time of the environmental shift.

The population is finite containing N individuals each possessing one variant of the cultural trait, either a or A. In each time step a new individual arises and adopts variant a or A , before replacing another randomly selected individual, who dies Initially, neither variant provides an adaptive benefit and both are transmitted from a randomly chosen role-model to a newborn with a probability equal to its frequency. In other words, the variants evolve neutrally, through unbiased transmission.

Thus, the variants a and A now evolve through payoff-biased transmission 5 and f can be interpreted as the cultural transmission advantage of variant a. Further, f quantifies the benefit of variant a after the environmental change and g the benefit of variant A assumed to be 1 in the following. It can be shown see Supplementary Section S1 in the supplementary material for a detailed derivation that the probability of fixation from a de novo innovation with adaptive benefit f is given by.

It holds see Supplementary Section S2 in the supplementary material for a detailed derivation that. Finally, we generalise the expression for the fixation probability 1 from a starting frequency of 1 to a general starting frequency of j using the fact that. So the probability of a fixation from standing cultural variation, at the time of an environmental change, i. Figure 1 illustrates those probabilities for various values of f.

The fixation probability is lowest if the adaptive trait is a de novo innovation, i. Before we discuss some implications of these results for the theory of cultural adaptation, we consider the influence of transmission processes other than unbiased transmission on the fixation probability. An important difference between genetic and cultural evolution is the large number of different ways in which information can be passed on from one generation to the next in a cultural context Cultural transmission processes affect how cultural traits are maintained or lost in a population 4.

As a result, it is possible that the probability of a sweep to fixation might depend on the cultural transmission processes on which a population relies before an environmental change. To quantify the effects of alternative transmission processes, we need to generalise Eq. Substituting these expressions in Eq. We note that the population still applies payoff-biased transmission after the environmental shift. In this case the transition probabilities are given by.

Calculating the fixation probability 4 using Eqs. Figure 1 shows that, compared to unbiased transmission see black solid line , conformity see short dashed line and anti-conformity see long dashed line show higher fixation probabilities for all values of f.

This is shown in Fig. The results so far have shown that if the standing cultural variation in a population contains a variant that becomes adaptive after an environmental change, then the probability of a sweep to fixation is higher compared to a situation where an adaptive variant with the same level of benefit is invented after an environmental shift.

These results suggest it is plausible that under some circumstances populations need not, and indeed should not, rely on inventing novel traits in novel environmental conditions if they possess adaptive standing variation. In other words, exploring the mechanisms that can generate standing variation containing an adaptive variant after an arbitrary environmental change is of great interest. An extensive analysis of these questions might require an n variant model to allow for the accumulation of cultural diversity and this is a subject of future research.

In the next section, as above, we explore in the two variant model a simple mechanism capable of generating and maintaining standing variation: foresight. We note that there are a number of candidate mechanisms capable of maintaining cultural variation such as frequent environmental changes, accurate copying of vast bodies of cultural knowledge, relatively accurate foresight, or high innovation rates.

Here, we investigate just one simple mechanism. So far we have assumed that standing variation, or the cultural repertoire, is maintained by unbiased or frequency-biased transmission.

There may be good reason to believe that the adaptive benefit of variants derived under such neutral conditions might be lower than variants invented in direct response to new environmental conditions. This means that a trade off may exist in a cultural system that does not exist in its genetic analogue: the adaptive value of cultural variants innovated under different circumstances might, on average, differ, with variants innovated in direct response to an environmental challenge having higher average adaptive value compared to those that are innovated prior to that challenge.

Here we allow for foresight, which might enable populations to invent or maintain variants in their cultural memory which could be of use after an environmental change. Therefore, we do not imply that the adaptive value of a variant innovated or maintained with foresight is always positive, nor that the generation of innovations is not random, only that the underlying distribution of fitness effects might be skewed by our cognitive abilities.

In the following we explore circumstances under which adaptation from de novo innovation is more likely, as a result of an increase in benefit associated with innovation in response to a challenge.

Figure 3 shows the difference in fixation probability. The difference in probability of a sweep from standing variation and a novel innovation with different strengths of ability for foresight y axis and directed innovation x axis. We assume that foresight cannot perform better than directed innovation and so consider the bottom diagonal only.

Directed innovations can only compensate for their frequency disadvantage if the level of benefit generated by standing variation is relatively small see black dots in Fig. This implies that adaptation from standing variation may prove more efficient than adaptation from de novo innovation over a wide parameter range and highlights the importance of understanding the level of adaptivness that can be generated in standing cultural variation.

Is it realistic to assume that standing variation can generate cultural variants that provide a high level of benefit after an arbitrary environmental change?

Or is it more likely that standing cultural variation proves to be only marginally adaptive by producing variants with a very small benefit? Is variation maintained in the standing cultural variation likely to be maladaptive? Above, we assumed that the level of cultural adaptation to changed environmental conditions has no influence on the survival of the population or on population demography more generally, since, by definition, the population size is constant at N even after an environmental change.

However, under this assumption, as we have seen in Fig. One clear answer may be population extinction Although it is not widely believed that organisms with large body size and long generation times are likely to undergo evolutionary rescue and remain, instead, the most vulnerable to extinction in changed environments 11 , 12 , we contend that humans are an exception.

In human populations genetic adaptation may be preceded by much more rapid cultural adaptation, as was the case in the smaller and faster breeding field cricket whose evolutionary rescue was accompanied by a facilitating behavioural adaptation In the case of humans, behavioural and cultural changes may be even more crucial. For example, extensive use of warm weather clothing is thought to have emerged as humans migrated away from the warmer climates in Africa and into Europe 24 some — kya However, at least some important genetic adaptations to the cold in European populations did not reach high frequencies before 3—8 kya This suggests the possibility that human populations may first survive extreme changes in their environments through behavioural and cultural adaptations, which may be followed by adaptive genetic changes.

This might also imply that if cultural adaptation to a novel environment does not succeed or does not proceed with sufficient speed, the population will face extinction As before, each individual in a model population is assumed to have one of two variants of a single cultural trait: an ancestral variant, A , or a novel innovation, a.

Neither of the two variants have an adaptive benefit in terms of transmission or survival before the shift. We assume that a cultural response to challenging environmental conditions may be more likely to prevent death rather than increase fertility taking, again, the example of cold weather clothing above.

To model this link between cultural adaptation and survival we relax the assumption of constant population size and define the following the birth-death process.

Each individual has a per-capita death rate depending on the adopted cultural variant. The value of r also indicates the initial level of maladaptation, a crucial parameter in determining the probability of population survival In each time step individuals die according to their death rate 8 and one naive individual is added to the population who adopts the variant from a chosen role model.

This choice is governed by the probabilities. In the following we focus on adaptation from de novo innovation, i. In other words, the spread of variant a , determines the fate of the population. If the fraction of the population with a increases fast enough, then the cultural adaptation successfully counteracts the increased mortality caused by the mismatch between the environment and the ancestral variant, A.

Figure 4 illustrates Eqs. An evolutionary rescue scenario where the ancestral variant causes excess mortality and the novel variant provides a survival benefit. Figure 5 A shows the interplay between the survival benefit q and the cultural transmission advantage f of the innovation a on the mean time to population rescue, i.

Parameter constellations shown in white do not result in a rescue. We observe a non-linear relationship between q and f : small values of q can be compensated for by large values of f and vice versa leading to a rapid rescue.

Figure 5 A shows four parameter domains. The domain labelled i contains the cases where cultural transmission bias makes a rescue possible by speeding up the spread of a weakly beneficial trait, where otherwise the population would collapse.

The domain labelled iv contains the values of q for which an evolutionary rescue is possible even in the absence of a transmission advantage to a. Finally, domains ii and iii contain cases where rescues are always possible or never possible and extinction is likely.

Figure 5 B shows the corresponding population bottlenecks, i. As expected, parameter constellations leading to a long rescue time also lead to small bottlenecks.

A The mean time to evolutionary rescue and B the severity of the population bottleneck. Simulations ran until the population was extinct or exceeded its original size, indicating a cultural evolutionary rescue in this parameter range. We can show that where the mismatch between the environment and the ancestral variant A is more pronounced, a rescue is less likely and where they do occur, the population bottleneck is more severe, as one might expect from previous work on evolutionary rescues 11 , 18 , Finally, we consider the cases in which population extinctions do occur despite the introduction of a beneficial cultural variant.

Here, we can show that a weakly beneficial variant, may, sometimes significantly, lengthen the time between the environmental change and population collapse see Fig.

In the presence of a beneficial cultural variant, this time to extinction can be increased considerably. The increase depends on the probability of spread of the cultural variant as well as the protection against death, quantified by q , that it confers compared to variant A.

This lends credence to the idea that the spread of a weakly beneficial cultural trait may facilitate true evolutionary rescue for human populations by prolonging the time a population can wait for a beneficial genetic mutation to arise and spread.

The time to population collapse, where cultural rescues do not occur, for different values of q. Threshold for extinction here is 5 individuals and is marked with a red solid line. Black and grey lines show total population sizes, red dashed lines show the frequency of the beneficial cultural trait. Higher frequencies of the beneficial trait delay population collapse—sometimes substantially. In summary, culture alone may be able to rescue a population, prevent or mitigate population bottlenecks, or extend the survival time of a declining population during which true evolutionary rescue may be possible in a way rare or unique for large, long-living organisms.

The uniquely well-developed cognitive and cultural abilities of humans have undoubtedly contributed to our success as a species. However, we understand little about the ways in which our ability to generate and maintain culture affect our adaptation to new environments and our probability of success in those environments.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000